The problem with explaining symptoms: The origin of biases in causal processing
Abstract
Understanding causation is complex, especially where it involves ‘the person’. Advances in physiological, psychosocial understanding and associated health interventions have nevertheless been made despite this complexity, but often in the face of a weak comprehension of the actual causal framework of any particular disorder. One of the problems highlighted by the CauseHealth Initiative and the European Society for Person Centered Healthcare is that our tools (e.g., the Scientific Method, Evidence-Based Medicine, Randomised Controlled Trials) are disappointingly weak in comparison with the model of understanding to which we aspire. Research from experimental psychology highlights a further constraint to our understanding; all animals have evolved neural mechanisms that solve natural causation in a manner that is similar to the scientific method. Our behaviour and thinking suffers from the same weaknesses as our methods. We will discuss experiments that have been conducted to test causal perceptions and mental representations. We address (1) a primary route for how causes are extracted from experience (2) the single cause bias and (3) representational complexity. These experiments have implications for both patient and practitioner as well as how they interact. Not only is the world more complex than we generally tend to acknowledge, but we have evolved to think more simply than we might wish.
Keywords
Full Text:
PDFReferences
Mumford, S. & Anjum, R.L. (2011). Getting causes from powers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Matute, H., Yarritu, I. & Vadillo, M.A. (2011). Illusions of causality at the heart of pseudoscience. British Journal of Psychology 102 (3) 392-405.
Msetfi, R.M., Kornbrot, D.E., Matute, H. & Murphy, R.A. (2015). The relationship between mood state and perceived control in contingency learning: effects of individualist and collectivist values. Frontiers in Psychology 6, 1430.
Oaksford, M. & Chater, N. (Eds.). (1998). Rational models of cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Murphy, R.A., Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Msetfi, R.M. & Baker, A.G. (2005). Signal-outcome contingency, contiguity and the depressive realism effect. In: New directions in associative learning, A.Wills (Ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Murphy, R.A., Drew, S. & Baker, A.G. (1998). Judging the importance of constant and variable candidate causes: A test of the Power PC theory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 51A, 65-84.
Vallée-Tourangeau, F. & Murphy, R.A. (1999). Action-effect contingency judgment tasks foster normative causal reasoning. In: Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, (820), M. Hahn & S.C. Stoness (Eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baker, A.G., Murphy, R.A. & Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (1996). Associative and normative models of causal induction: Reacting to versus understanding cause. In: The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, D.R. Shanks, K.J. Holyoak & D.L. Medin (Eds.), Vol. 34, pp. 1-45. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Murphy, R.A., Mondragon, E. & Murphy, V.A. (2009). Contingency, temporal order and generalization: Building blocks of causal cognition. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 22, 61-74.
Shanks, D.R. (1987). Acquisition functions in contingency judgment. Learning and Motivation 18 (2) 147-166.
Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary animal learning theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Murphy, R.A. & Baker, A.G. (2004). A role for CS-US contingency in Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 30, 229-239.
Rescorla, R.A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It's not what you think it is. American Psychologist 43 (3) 151.
Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Murphy, R.A. & Baker, A.G. (1998). Causal induction in the presence of a perfect inhibitory cue: Contrasting predictions from the Rescorla-Wagner model and the Pearce model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 51B, 173-191.
Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Hollingsworth, L. & Murphy, R.A. (1998). ‘Attentional bias’ in correlation judgements? Smedslund (1963) revisited. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 39, 221-233.
Marks, G. & Miller, N. (1987). Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: An empirical and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin 102 (1) 72.
Murphy, R.A., Schmeer, S., Vallee-Tourangeau, F., Mondragon, E. & Hilton, D. (2011). Making the illusory correlation appear and then disappear: The effect of more learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 64, 24-40.
Cavus, H.A. & Msetfi, R.M. (2016). The Effects of Cognitive Load during Intertrial Intervals on Judgements of Control: The Role of Working Memory and Contextual Learning. Acta Psychologica 171, 47-56.
Atkinson, R.C. & Estes, W.K. (1962). Stimulus sampling theory. In: Handbook of Mathematical Psychology, R.D. Luce, R.R. Bush & G. Eugene (Eds.). Oxford: Wiley.
Rescorla, R., & Wagner, A. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In: Classical conditioning II: Theory and research, A. Black & W. Prokasy (Eds.), pp. 64-99. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.
Collins, D.J. & Shanks, D.R. (2006). Summation in causal learning: Elemental processing or configural generalization? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (9) 1524-1534.
Rescorla, R.A. (1997). Summation: Assessment of a configural theory. Animal Learning & Behavior 25 (2) 200-209.
Soto, F.A., Vogel, E.H., Castillo, R.D. & Wagner, A.R. (2009). Generality of the summation effect in human causal learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (5) 877-889.
Grand, C. & Honey, R.C. (2008). Solving Xor. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 34 (4) 486.
Williams, D.A., Sagness, K.E. & McPhee, J.E. (1994). Configural and elemental strategies in predictive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20 (3) 694-709.
Byrom, N.C. & Murphy, R.A. (2014). Sampling Capacity Underlies Individual Differences in Human Associative Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 40 (2) 133-143.
Byrom, N.C. & Murphy, R.A. (2016). Individual difference in configural associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 42 (4) 325.
Basso, M.R., Schefft, B.K., Ris, M.D. & Dember, W.N. (1996). Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 2 (3) 249-255.
Fredrickson, B.L. & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion 19 (3) 313-332.
Gasper, K. & Clore, G.L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science 13 (1) 34-40.
Rowe, G., Hirsh, J.B. & Anderson, A.K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 (1) 383-388.
Seligman, M.E. & Maier, S.F. (1967). Failure to Escape Traumatic Shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology 74 (1) 1-9.
Alloy, L.B. & Abramson, L.Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108 (4) 441.
Msetfi, R.M., Murphy, R.A. & Kornbrot, D. (2012). Effect of mild depression on time discrimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65, 632-645.
Msetfi, R.M., Murphy, R.A., Simpson, J. & Kornbrot, D.E. (2005). Depressive realism and outcome density bias in contingency judgements: The effect of context and the inter-trial interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 134 (1) 10-22.
Byrom, N., Msetfi, R. & Murphy, R. (2015). Two pathways to causal control: use and availability of information in the environment in people with and without signs of depression. Acta Psychologica 157, 1-12.
Msetfi, R.M., Wade, C. & Murphy, R.A. (2013). Context and time in causal learning: Contingency and mood dependent effects. PLoS ONE 8 (5) e64063.
Msetfi, R.M., Murphy, R.A. & Simpson, J. (2007). Depressive realism and the effect of intertrial interval on judgements of zero, positive, and negative contingencies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 60 (3) 461-481.
Msetfi, R.M., Cavus, H.A. & Brosnan, L. (2016). Enhanced Attention to Context Increases Perceived Control in Mild Depression. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (6) 1073-1081.
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5750/ejpch.v5i3.1318
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.