Open Access Open Access  Restricted Access Subscription Access

Updating the descriptive biopsychosocial approach to fit into a formal person-centered dynamic coherence model - Part III: Personhood, salutogenesis and further topics

Thomas Frölich, F F Bevier, A Babakhani, h H Chisholm, P Henningsen, D S Miall, S Sandberg, A Schmitt

Abstract


In the present paper the approach, as outlined in our previous articles, is applied to a range of subjects. Its main goal is to understand how human specifics such as use of language, cultural creativity, rational thinking and the use of abstractive terms interfere with human beings basic organic processes. This necessitates an examination at the level of semantics. The latter is referred to as the relationship of produced states to a meaning so that from a sender’s side they are meant to transport meaning from a receiver. Since an interaction of two distinct entities is at issue here, we have to describe the interaction in formal terms, so we talk about the hypostasising of a triggering contact of one entity’s change of states initiating a change of states of another entity. This contact can be accepted as a trigger or not as a trigger, a decision that corresponds to the most primitive form of (possibly also mutual) interpretation. With this model in mind, we can interpret different communicative settings, as the monomeric self-efficacy interpretation, or mutual interpretations in families. To interpret needs a physically memorised categorisation. Only this allows for fast and set-related attributions. Such categorisations can go wrong, especially in fast changing environments. If a higher resolution is needed, additional distinctions are implemented into the heuristic interpretative procedures. Human beings have additional tools for higher resolution in differentiation: practical segmentation and speaking. Afterwards, the products of material and logical discretisation may be re-combined into a new arrangement. The corresponding accessory potential helps to generate a world of its own, as a full-grown personhood. Human uniqueness hence is based on the individuality of each person’s material basis, as well as on its individual type of differentiation and corresponding attribution. Being aware of this, the also mental individuality allows to understand self-interpretative processes that support salutogenesis and an experience of being self-efficient, or not. 


Keywords


Actuality, biopsychosocial model, discretisation, dynamic coherence providers (DCPs), epistemology, formalisation of meaning, hermeneutics, humanities, informatics, narratology, non-Cartesian approach, ontology, personal individuality

Full Text:

PDF

References


Fröhlich, T., Bevier, F.F., Babakhani, A., Chisholm, H.H., Henningsen, P., Miall, D.S., Sandberg, S. & Schmitt, S. (2016). Updating the descriptive biopsychosocial approach to fit into a formal person-centered dynamic coherence model - Part I: Some few basics. European Journal for Person Centered Healthcare 4 (3) 548-556.

Fröhlich, T., Bevier, F.F., Babakhani, A., Chisholm, H.H., Henningsen, P., Miall, D.S., Sandberg, S. & Schmitt, S. (2016). Updating the descriptive biopsychosocial approach to fit into a formal person-centered dynamic coherence model - Part II: Applications and some more basics. European Journal for Person Centered Healthcare 4 (3) 557-566.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-Efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change. Psychological Review 84 (2) 191 -215.

Antonovsky, A. (1979) Health, stress, and coping. San Francisco, London: Jossey-Bass.

Werner, E.E., Bierman, J.M. & French, F.E. (1971). The children of Kauai: a longitudinal study from the prenatal period to age ten. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

Winnicott, D.W. (2000). The Child, the Family, and the Outside World. London: Penguin Books.

Pietrowicz, S. (1992). Helmuth Plessner, Genese und System seiner philosophisch-anthropologischen Denkens. Freiburg und Munchen: Alber-Verlag.

de Mul, J. (ed.). (2014). Plessner's philosophical anthropology: perspectives and prospects. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press,

Radke-Uhlmann, G. (ed.). (2012). Phronesis - die Tugend der Geisteswissenschaften, Heidelberg: Universitatsverlag Winter.

Shannon, C.E. (1948). A Mathematical Theory of Communication. Bell System Technical Journal 27 (3) 379-423.

Wikipedia, (2016). https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Entropy_%28information_theory%29. Last accessed 06 February 2016.

LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York: Touchstone.

Miall, D.S. (2007). Feeling from the Perspective of the Empirical Study of Literature. Journal of Literary Theory 1 (2) 377-393.

Miall, D.S. (1989). Beyond the schema given: Affective comprehension of literary narratives. Cognition and Emotion 3, 55-78.

Sachs, J. (1995). Aristotle’s Physics: a Guided Study. London: Rutgers University Press.

Coleridge, S.T. (1983). Biographia literaria [1817], 2 vols. Engell, J. & Jackson Bate. W., (eds.). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Kidd, D.C. & Castano, E. (2013). Reading Literary Fiction Improves Theory of Mind, Science, Sciencexpress, 3 October 2013 / Page 1 /. Available at: http://science.sciencemag.org/content/342/6156/377.

Mukhopadhyay, D. (2014). Understanding the neuropsychology of aesthetic paradox: the dual phase oscillation hypothesis. Review of General Psychology 18, 237-248.

Cela-Conde, C.J., Garcia-Prieto, J., Ramasco, J.J., Mirasso, O.R., Bajo, R., Munar E., Flexas, A., del-Pozo, F. & Maestu, F. (2013). Dynamics of brain networks in the aesthetic appreciation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA (PNAS), 10454-10461, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302855110.

Mukhopadhyay, D. (2015). The Dual Phase Oscillation Hypothesis and the Neuropsychology of Docu-Fiction Film. Consciousness, Literature and the Arts 16, 1.

https://blackboard.lincoln.ac.uk/bbcswebdav/users/dmeyerdinkgrafe/archive/mukhopadhyay.pdf.

Ding, N., Melloni, L, Zhang, H., Tian, X. & Poeppel, D., (2016). Cortical tracking of hierarchical linguistic structures in connected speech. Nature Neuroscience 19, 158-164.

Golumbic, E.M.Z., Ding, N., Bickel, S., Lakatos, P., Schevon, C.A., McKhann, G.M., Goodman, R.R., Emerson, R., Mehta, A.D., Simon, J.Z., Poepple, D. & Schroeder, C.E. (2013). Mechanisms underlying selective neuronal tracking of attended speech at a “cocktail party”. Neuron 77 (5) 980-991.

Alcaro, A., Huber, R. & Panksepp, J. (2007). Behavioral functions of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system: An affective neuroethological perspective. Brain Research Reviews 56 (2) 283-321.

Westbury, C., Shaoul, C., Moroschan, G. & Ramscar, M. (2016). Telling the world’s least funny jokes: On the qualification of humour as entropy. Journal of Memory and Language 86, 141-156.

Jonze, S. (1999). Being John Malkovich. Universal City: Universal City Studios Productions LLLP.




DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5750/ejpch.v4i3.1214

Refbacks

  • There are currently no refbacks.